How many colors in the world




















Then, we build a model for that tree of how different features in this case, color terms are gained or lost, and how quickly those features might change. This is a complicated problem; we estimate likely reconstructions, evaluate that model for how well it fits our hypotheses, tweak the model parameters a bit to produce a different set of results, score that model, and so on.

We repeat this many times millions of times, usually and then take a random sample of our estimates. This method is due originally to evolutionary biologists Mark Pagel and Andrew Meade. Our results supported some of the previous findings, but questioned others. For the most part, our color data showed that Australian languages also show the patterns of color term naming that have been proposed elsewhere in the world; if there are three named colors, they will be black, white and red not, for example, black, white and purple.

But we show that it is most likely that Australian languages have lost color terms, as well as gained them. This contradicts 40 years of assumptions of how color terms change — and makes color words look a lot more like other words.

We also looked at where the color words themselves came from. Some were old in the family, and seemed to go back as color terms. So Australian languages show similar sources of color terms to languages elsewhere in the world: color words change when people draw analogies with items in their environment.

Our research shows the potential for using language change to study areas of science that have previously been more closely examined by fields such as psychology. Then you split your set in half and guess again. The number of guesses it takes the ideal listener to zero in on my color chip based on the color word I used is a simple score for the chip. Using these scores, we can now rank the colors across the grid, in any language.

In English, it turns out that people can convey the warm colors — reds, oranges and yellows — more efficiently with fewer guesses than the cool colors — blues and greens.

The left-to-right ordering is from easiest to communicate fewest guesses needed to get the right color to hardest to communicate. The diagram shows that all languages have roughly the same order, with the warm colors on the left easy to communicate and the cool ones on the right harder to communicate. This generalization occurs in spite of the fact that languages near the bottom of the figure have few terms that people use consistently, while languages near the top like English and Spanish have many terms that most people use consistently.

In addition to discovering this remarkable universal across languages, we also wanted to find out what causes it. Recall that our idea is that maybe we introduce words into a language when there is something that we want to talk about. So perhaps this effect arises because objects — the things we want to talk about — tend to be warm-colored. We evaluated this hypothesis in a database of 20, photographs of objects that people at Microsoft had decided contained objects, as distinct from backgrounds.

This data set is available to train and test computer vision systems that are trying to learn to identify objects.

Our colleagues then determined the specific boundaries of the object in each image and where the background was. Yes, those are the colors that make up the rainbow as we know it. However, there is a hidden world of color that the human brain can barely comprehend coming at us from all directions. All of the other colors in the world are technically just combinations of these core colors.

Researchers who study color are pretty confident that there is a cap on the total possible number of perceivable colors in the world. The way to figure out how many colors exist in the world is to start with how many shades of light the human eye can actually see. According to researchers , the answer is 1, shades of light. Within those shades, we can detect different levels of red-green shades.

The short answers: Isaac Newton. And Ancient Greek philosophy. Um, what? The visual spectrum — Color theory is a bit more complicated than stirring together the right finger paints. Subtractive color mixing is pretty close to the paint mixing we did in grade school. Additive color mixing. If you like me have a hard time wrapping your head around how red and green mix together to make yellow, watch this YouTube video. You're in! You proved us right again.

Our newsletter is only for the coolest kids. Get ready for amazing stuff in your inbox. Looks like something went wrong. I'm a freelance designer Our newsletter is for everyone who loves design!

Sign me up! Join the community. Current design contests Designers, check out these contests so you can start building your career.



0コメント

  • 1000 / 1000